Examining the gender identity of language teachers using a masculinity-femininity scale: A case from Iran
Reza Pishghadam, Fahime Saboori, Laila Samavarchi and Tahereh Hassanzadeh
Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Iran
The present study pursued two goals: first, to construct and validate a masculinity/femininity scale (MFS); and second, to reveal and compare the dominant gender identity of English, Arabic, and Persian teachers. Regarding the first goal, a 30-item gender identity scale was designed and, using the data collected from 300 junior high school students, its construct validity was substantiated through Structural Equation Modeling. As for the second goal, an additional 623 students rated their 130 teachers. The whole data was then analysed using Chi-square testing. The findings were indicative of the dominant femininity of English teachers and masculinity of Arabic and Persian teachers. Also, based on the results, English teachers were found to be more feminine and less masculine than Arabic and Persian teachers. Finally, statistical results were discussed, and implications were provided for language teaching in the formal context of education.
In the same vein, another factor which may impact the quality of teachers and hence make a contribution to a more efficient teacher education is the concept of masculinity and femininity. Also referred to as gender identity, this concept forms one of the most basic and powerful components of personality (Bem, 1974) and is defined as the self-perception of maleness and femaleness given what it means to be a man or a woman in society (Stets & Burke, 2000). The common trend found among different societies considers men to be assertive, tough, and competitive and women to be modest, caring, and cooperative (Hofstede, 1980). As for educational settings, it seems that teachers who are more corrective and concerned with the excellence of the students and foster competition in the class are regarded as masculine teachers, whereas those who are more permissive, promote cooperation and social skills, and value rapport with students are regarded as feminine ones. Examining this feature of teachers with respect to the subject matter they teach can lead to insightful educational implications. With regard to the potential of this concept in enriching teacher education especially for language teachers, this paper seeks first to construct a masculinity/femininity scale (MFS) for teachers and next to reveal the gender identity of language teachers as perceived by students.
First, there is no theoretically straight-forward way to distinguish between typical male and female behaviours (Sternberg, 1993). In other words, rather than being innate or a function of biological differences, masculinity and femininity result from social and cultural conditions and are formed through gender role socialisation beginning in the family and continuing through religious and educational institutions, mass media, and peer networks (Katz, 1986; Hofstede, Hofstede, &Minkov, 2010; Sifuna, Chege, & Oanda, 2006; Stromquist, 2007). This issue was first addressed by the anthropologist Margaret Mead in Sex and temperament in three primitive societies (1935). This early study illustrated that the self-meanings regarding one's gender are shaped by the cultural expectations held for each sex, and needless to say, these expectations differ from one society to other. In spite of the great deal of varieties across cultures on this issue, some universals can be found. Generally speaking, males tend to be more aggressive, dominant, active, competitive, and instrumental, whereas females are more likely to be warm, submissive, passive, cooperative, and expressive (Persson, 1999; Stets & Burke, 2000). Seemingly, such stereotypical self-perceptions are less common among men and women in more socially and economically developed countries (Williams & Best, 1989).
The second point is that, contrary to the traditional view, gender identity is not restricted to a dichotomy. Constantinople (1973) was the pioneer in challenging the mutually exclusive view of gender. She argued that rather than representing two opposite ends of a single scale, masculinity and femininity are in fact two distinct dimensions on which individuals could be measured. That is, a person could, at the same time, be high or low in both masculinity and femininity. Such a two-dimensional concept of gender resulted in the coinage of the term androgyny (from 'andro' meaning male, and 'gyn' referring to female) by Bem (1974) to encompass the gender identity of those who possess both masculine and feminine qualities, depending on which behaviours best suit a particular situation. In addition, while the common view labelled those who did not fit within the masculine-feminine divide as deviant and ostracised (Chege & Sifuna, 2006), Bem (1974) boldly presented androgyny as an advantage due to offering greater behavioural flexibility.
This debate over the single or two dimensional view of gender identity was also embraced by Geert Hofstede (1980), one of the key figures in intercultural studies, who presented masculinity/femininity as one of the dimensions constituting his four-dimensional cultural model. Hofstede used the term masculinity to capture certain propensities like the degree to which dominant values in a society tend to be assertive, competitive, ego-oriented, tough, aggressive, and non-emotional. He also used the term feminine for cultures whose dominant values were modest, nurturing, passive, emotional, submissive, cooperative, and receptive. Regarding the question of whether masculinity/femininity is one dimension or two, he argued that an individual can be both masculine and feminine at the same time; however, a country culture is usually predominantly one or the other.
Hofstede et al. (2010) discussed important manifestations of femininity and masculinity in teaching and education. According to them, feminine teachers tend to be more tender, modest, and caring, and put a higher premium on their relationship with the students through building rapport with them. That is, teachers' friendliness and social skills are appreciated in feminine education. In the same vein, students' social adaptation is emphasised through utilising methods that foster solidarity and cooperation among students. Moreover, this type of education is permissive in the sense that mistakes are generally treated as an integral part of learning, there is not just one right answer and relativism has a special place, the average student is considered the norm, and teachers will rather praise weaker students to encourage them. In masculine education, on the other hand, teachers are more likely to be tough, assertive, and ego-oriented. The characteristic that is most admired in a teacher is his or her brilliance and academic reputation. Masculine teachers tend to promote a competitive atmosphere in the class in which students' academic performance and excellence is stressed. Finally, masculine education is corrective, meaning that accuracy is highly valued in it, there is only one right answer, and the best student is considered the norm and is commonly praised.
There is ample empirical literature investigating gender identity in the educational context. A cursory look into this body of research reveals such major themes as gendered curriculum, i.e. associating different subject matters with femininity and masculinity (e.g., Francis, 2000; Chapman, 2000; Paechter, 2001), the role of educational institutions in gender role socialisation (Bigler, Hayes, & Hamilton, 2013; Kangethe, Lyria & Nyamanga, 2014; Stromquist, 2007), gender stereotyping in subject choices and educational opportunities (Favara, 2012; Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2004; Kimura, 2000), the relationship between gender identity and academic performance (e.g. Marrs, Sigler & Brammer, 2012; Paver & Gammie, 2005; Wheeless & Potorti, 1989), and the impact of gender orientation on reading and writing motivations (Pajares & Valiante, 2001; McGeown, Goodwin, Henderson & Wright, 2012 ).
Nevertheless, this rich literature has seemingly ignored a promising research area, namely, teachers' gender identity. That is, researchers have hardly ever probed into the perceived notions and implications of masculinity and femininity associated with teachers and many scholars have criticised teacher education for failing to bridge this gap yet (berg, 2008; Braun, 2011; Meyenn & Parker, 2001; Younger & Warrington, 2008; Weiner, 2005).
Investigation of this issue for language teachers could be of particular significance in the Iranian educational context. In this country, there are three languages taught in the formal system of education: Persian, as the formal and the native language of the country; Arabic, as the language of religion (Islam); and English, as the language of international communication. The distinct and somehow divergent role of each in the curriculum has prompted researchers to compare corresponding teachers in different respects including status (Pishghadam & Saboori, 2014) and stroke (Irajzad, 2015). The former study revealed that English teachers had the highest status among language teachers in the students' perception and the latter showed that English teachers got the second rank, after Arabic teachers, in providing students with strokes (namely, every action to acknowledge other's presence; Berne, 1988). In the same vein and with regard to the paucity of research on teachers' gender identity, the present study aims to construct a MFS for teachers and investigate the masculinity and femininity of the teachers of these three languages as perceived by the students.
The scale included 30 adjectives pertaining to the three components of gender identity, i.e. masculinity, femininity, and androgyny (10 to each), to be marked for the teachers of the three languages, Persian, Arabic and English. The scale was in Persian, the mother tongue of the participants (See Appendix for the English translation). Masculinity was represented by adjectives such as kind, intelligent, friendly, etc.; Androgyny by open-minded, high-class, well-dressed, etc.; and Femininity by knowledgeable, well-educated, consultable, etc.
Three reasons prompted the researchers to construct the scale rather than simply using the most known scale on gender identity, i.e. the BSRI (Bem, 1974). Firstly, it is evident that understanding of gender identity and behaviour is not the same across different cultures (Persson, 1999). Hence, the results obtained by the BSRI, which has been originally developed for the American culture, might not be as valid for a culture such as the Persian. Secondly, as this research was to be conducted in the educational context and on school students, the instrument needed to include the psychometric properties which are acceptable for this context, hold true for teachers, and are at the same time easily intelligible for students. Thirdly, the format of the BSRI (consisting of 60 adjectives to be numbered on a scale from 1 to7) did not suit the target participants of this study.
In the process of collecting the data, first, the designed MFS was subject to pilot-testing in order to disambiguate the items and ensure the content validity of the scale. Next, the revised version of the scale was administered to 300 participants and the data was used to substantiate the construct validity of the scale. Finally, an additional 623 students completed the MFS.
The data analysis entailed two phases. In the first phase, structural equation modeling (SEM) via AMOS version 18 was utilised to substantiate the construct validity of the MFS. Next, the internal consistency of the whole scale as well as the reliability of each factor constructing the validated scale was assessed using the Cronbach alpha reliability estimate. In the second phase, Chi-square (using SPSS version 19) was employed to see whether the differences between the gender identities of the teachers were significant.
The goodness of fit measures in AMOS were used to examine the viability of the hypothesised model for the CDS. Chi-square/degree of freedom (χ2/df), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and Root Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) were the goodness of fit indices utilised in this study. To have an acceptable fit model, χ2/df should be less than 3, AGFI, IFI, TLI, and CFI should be above .90, and RMSEA should be less than .08 (MacCallum, Browne & Sugawara, 1996). Results of the CFA indicated that all the goodness-of-fit indices were above the cutoff points (see Table 4). Therefore, the CFA confirmed the factor structure of CDS. The Cronbach alpha estimated the reliability of all the items as .92. All of the three factors yielded good reliability estimates ranging from .88 to .91. None of the items were removed after examining the outcome of the factor rotation.
Fit index | χ2/df | AGFI | IFI | TLI | CFI | RMSEA |
Acceptable range | < 3 2.47 | > 90 0.97 | 0.90 | 0.93 | 0.94 | < 0.08 0.05 |
Figure 1: The result of the SEM
English | Arabic | Persian | ||||
Observed N | Expected N | Observed N | Expected N | Observed N | Expected N | |
Masculinity | 2687 | 2800.7 | 3019 | 2684.3 | 2797 | 2670.0 |
Androgyny | 2757 | 2800.7 | 2599 | 2684.3 | 2614 | 2670.0 |
Femininity | 2958 | 2800.7 | 2435 | 2684.3 | 2599 | 2670.0 |
Chi-square Asymp. Sig. | 14.133 .001 | 67.596 .000 | 9.103 .011 |
Table 2 indicates the dominant factor in each teacher. While there was no significant difference between English teachers' masculinity and androgyny, the difference between these two and their femininity turned out to be significant. In other words, students considered English teachers to be significantly (χ2=14.133, p<.05) more feminine than masculine or androgynous (Femininity>Masculinity / Androgyny).
Arabic teachers, in contrast, turned out to have dominant masculinity. That is, the significant difference occurred between masculinity and the combination of androgyny and femininity. They were accordingly perceived by the students to be significantly (χ2=67.596, p<.05) more masculine than feminine or androgynous (Masculinity > Androgyny / Femininity).
For Persian teachers, the dominant factor was again found to be masculinity. As Table 2 suggests, the difference between femininity and androgyny was not significant but there was a significant difference between these two and masculinity. Hence, similar to Arabic teachers, Persian teachers were significantly (χ2=9.103, p<.05) considered to be more masculine than androgynous or feminine (Masculinity>Androgyny / Femininity).
Masculinity | Androgyny | Femininity | ||||
Observed N | Expected N | Observed N | Expected N | Observed N | Expected N | |
English | 2687 | 2834.3 | 2757 | 2656.7 | 2958 | 2664.0 |
Arabic | 3019 | 2834.3 | 2599 | 2656.7 | 2435 | 2664.0 |
Persian | 2797 | 2834.3 | 2614 | 2656.7 | 2599 | 2664.0 |
Chi-square Asymp. Sig. | 20.182 .000 | 5.726 .057 | 53.595 .000 |
As for the second line of analysis, namely, the comparison of the language teachers in each factor, Table 3 shows the results of the Chi-square test for Masculinity as the first factor. Based on the table, a significant difference existed between the three teachers in this factor (χ2=20.182, p<.05). Moreover, Arabic and Persian teachers were found to be the more masculine than English teachers (Arabic / Persian > English).
Regarding androgyny, however, the results presented in this table revealed no significant difference (χ2=5.726, p>.05) between the three groups (English /Arabic / Persian).
With respect to femininity, as the third factor, the difference between English teachers and the other two groups turned out to be significant (χ2=53.595, p<.05). It follows that, English teachers were, in students' eyes, significantly more feminine than Arabic or Persian teachers (English >Arabic / Persian).
Masculinity | ||
Observed N | Expected N | |
English | 8402 | 8155.0 |
Arabic | 8053 | 8155.0 |
Persian | 8010 | 8155.0 |
Chi-square Asymp. Sig. | 11.335 .003 |
Finally, Table 4 illustrates the results of the overall evaluation of the teachers - all three factors combined - by the students. Once more, the results suggested a significant difference between the English teachers and the other two groups of teachers (χ2=11.335, p<.05). Simply put, English teachers received significantly more attention from the students as compared with Arabic and Persian teachers (English >Arabic / Persian).
With respect to the first goal, a three-factor model of MFS with 30 items was designed based on Bem's (1974) theory of gender identity. SEM was applied to substantiate the construct validity and reliability of the scale in the context of Iranian schools. The results of the goodness-of-fit indices showed a sufficient fit to the data confirming the factor structure of MFS. On such grounds, it can be claimed that MFS can be considered as an efficient scale for measuring the gender identity of Iranian teachers.
Regarding the second goal, the results of the Chi-square tests denoted the femininity of the English teachers and masculinity of the Arabic and Persian teachers in the students' perceptions. These results were also indicative of the higher femininity and lower masculinity of the English teachers as compared to the Arabic and Persian teachers.
There are some lines of explanation for these findings. First off, an overview of the English teaching practice in Iran would highlight the prevalence of communicative methods in English classes with a premium on teacher's rapport and relationship with, and care for learners (Pishghadam, 2011) The activities common in these classes such as pairing or group work, and discussions in which learners get to express themselves freely, foster social skills and underscore fluency at the expense of accuracy (Pishghadam, Zabihi, & Shayesth, 2013). Clearly, all these features accord with a feminine role's permissiveness, solidarity, and emphasis on the relationship with others.
Secondly, Arabic has a distinct status in this country; it is taught as the language of the religion. With an eye toward the acknowledged masculinity of the religion it represents, i.e. Islam and of the culture it is interwoven with (Hofstede, 1980), it would not be surprising to infer the masculinity of the language itself. This can be further justified by the corrective mood prevalent in Arabic classes in Iranian schools (Irajzad, 2015). Some other masculine features common in these classes include strict teachers' emphasis on accuracy and the existence of just one right answer.
As for the Persian classes, it should be remembered that, unlike the previous two, language is not taught in its totality in these classes. This is because Persian is the mother tongue of the students and they already know it well. But what is actually taught is some newly coined vocabularies to replace 'loan' vocabularies and, more importantly, prescriptive grammar to promote the 'correct' way of speaking. The high stress on the accuracy is in part due to the fact that the Persian language represents and enhances the national identity of the students. This issue is of particular importance with regard to these students' belief in the superiority of English over Persian and Arabic and their more powerful western identities compared with their national and religious identities (Pishghadam & Saboori, 2014). Hence, considering the close link between language learning and identity construction (Norton, 2000), and in order to enhance national identity and resist western linguistic imperialism, Persian teachers are obliged to put a high premium on the instruction and use of the correct grammar and purely Persian words in these classes. Not surprisingly, then, this prescriptive approach comes along with masculinity.
Still another noteworthy point indicated by the results was that, in the students' perception, the teachers were either masculine or feminine and none was found to be androgynous. Nor was there any significant difference among the teachers in their androgyny. This finding could allude to the persistence of the dichotomous view of gender among these students. This could in turn result from the prevalence of a non-relative (black and white) view among Iranian students (Pishghadam & Saboori, 2011; Pishghadam & Sabouri, 2011) endorsing, in part, Pishghadam and Mirzaee's (2009) claim that Iran's educational system still lives in the modernist era.
Also, the last finding of this study revealed that students have paid greater attention in evaluating their English teachers than their Arabic and Persian teachers. This could imply the greater importance attached to the English language than to the Arabic and Persian languages from the point of view of the students. This is in line with Pishghadam and Saboori (2014) illustrating that English teachers have a higher status than Persian and Arabic teachers in the students' perception.
All in all, our findings seem to contradict Bense (2014) in which the first part of the title is Languages aren't as important here, where 'here' is Australia. While Bense indicates that, from a German teachers' perspective, languages and language education are undervalued in the Australian context, this paper illustrates the unique significance of each language taught in the Iranian context with regard to its gender identity. This could, in turn, imply the role and contribution of a cultural context in highlighting languages.
To conclude, the present study revealed and compared the dominant gender identity of English, Arabic, and Persian school teachers. This can be affected in part by the teachers' idiosyncrasies but more importantly by the subject matter and the approach taken in teaching it. Accordingly, the findings can contribute to the current literature on teacher education. Furthermore, these findings should be infused in teacher pre- and in-service training courses which address the practical needs of language teachers. It may be beneficial to raise their awareness of the dominant gender identities and their manifestations and implications in teaching languages. Moreover, putting this information in access for applicants to language teacher preparation programs and for those who recruit teachers may help attract, in the first place, candidates who possess the corresponding gender identity.
The other attainment of this study was the construction of the MFS for the measurement of teachers' gender identity. To our best knowledge, this is the first gender identity scale specifically designed for an educational context and appropriate for the Persian culture. Future research can utilise this scale in exploring the masculinity and femininity of teachers of other subjects at different levels of education. This scale can also be utilised in comparing the dominant gender identity of language teachers in the formal and informal contexts of education. Moreover, further research could enrich teacher education by examining the relationship between teachers' gender identity and their efficacy. Finally, in order to conduct a more comprehensive examination of teachers' gender identity, other methods such as interview and observation should also be employed. Finally, this study was confined to measuring and comparing the gender identity of the language teachers. Future studies are recommended to delve into each type of gender identity (masculinity, femininity, and androgyny) separately, investigating its significant manifestations and implications for the context of language education.
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0036215
Bense, K. (2014). Languages aren't as important here: German migrant teachers' experiences in Australian language classes. The Australian Educational Researcher, 41(4), 485-497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13384-014-0143-2
Berne, E. (1988). Games people play. Penguin: UK.
Bigler, R. S., Hayes, A. R. & Hamilton, V. (2013). The role of schools in the early socialization of gender difference. In R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin & R. Peters (Eds.), Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development and Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development. http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/sites/default/files/textes-experts/en/2492/the-role-of-schools-in-the-early-socialization-of-gender-differences.pdf
Black, R. S. & Howard-Jones, A. (2000). Reflections on best and worst teachers: An experiential perspective of teaching. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 34(1), 1-12.
Braun, A. (2011). 'Walking yourself around as a teacher': Gender and embodiment in student teachers' working lives. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 32(2), 275-291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01425692.2011.547311
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Chapman, A. (2000). The difference it has made: The impact of the women's movement on education. Independent School, 60(1), 20-30.
Chege, F. N. & Sifuna, D. N. (2006). Girls' and women's education in Kenya: Gender perspectives and trends. Nairobi: UNESCO. http://library.unesco-iicba.org/English/Girls%20Education/All%20Articles/
General/Girls%20and%20womens%20education%20in%20Kenya.pdf
Constantinople, A. (1973). Masculinity-femininity: An exception to a famous dictum? Psychological Bulletin, 80(5), 389-407. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0035334
Covino, E. A. & Inwanicki, E. F. (1996). Experienced teachers: Their constructs of effective teaching. Journal of Personnel Evaluation in Education, 10(4), 325-363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00125499
Cheung, L. E., Cheng, M. M. & Pang, K. C. (2008). Building a model to define the concept of teacher success in Hong Kong. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24(3), 623-634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2007.09.007
Favara, M. (2012). The cost of acting "girly": Gender stereotypes and educational choices. IZA Discussion Papers 7037, Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA). http://ftp.iza.org/dp7037.pdf
Francis, B. (2000). Boys, girls and achievement. Addressing the classroom issues. London: RoutledgeFalmer.
Galluzzo, G. R. (2005). Performance assessment and renewing teacher education: The possibilities of the NBPTS standards. The Clearing House, 78(4), 142?145. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/TCHS.78.4.142-145
Ghanizadeh, A. & Moafian, F. (2011). The relationship between Iranian EFL teachers' sense of self-efficacy and their pedagogical success in language institutes. The Asian EFL Journal, 13(2), 249-272. http://asian-efl-journal.com/565/quarterly-journal/2011/06/the-relationship-between-iranian-efl-teachers-sense-of-self-efficacy-andtheir-pedagogical-success-in-language-institutes/
Hashemi, M. (2008). On the role of teachers' emotional intelligence on their pedagogical success. Unpublished master's thesis. Tehran: Allame Tabataba'I University.
Hedlin, M. (2013). Teachers and school discipline 1960-1970: Constructions of femininities and masculinities in Teachers' Journal. Education Inquiry, 4(4), 755-773. http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/edui.v4i4.23220
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE.
Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J. & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hunter, S. T. (2014). Iran, Islam, and the struggle for identity and power in the Islamic Republic of Iran. ACMCU Occasional paper. Georgetown University. https://issuu.com/georgetownsfs/docs/shireen_hunter_iran__islam__and__th
Ingvarson, L., Elliott, A., Kleinhenz, E. & McKenzie, P. (2006). Teacher education accreditation: A review of national and international trends and practices. Melbourne: Australian Institute for Teaching and School Leadership Ltd. http://www.aitsl.edu.au/research-and-evaluation/aitsl-research-repository/detail/?id=teacher-education-accreditation-a-review-of-national-and-international-trends-and-practices
Irajzad, F. (2015). Stroke analysis of Persian, Arabic, and English teachers: A case of female teachers in the context of Iranian schools. Unpublished master`s thesis. Mashhad: Ferdowsi University.
Kangethe, N. S., Lyria, K. S. & Nyamanga, A. M. (2014). The gender socialization question in education: Influence on boys' and girls' academic achievement. European Scientific Journal, 10(9), 279-294. http://eujournal.org/index.php/esj/article/viewFile/3802/3618
Katz, P. A. (1986). Gender identity: Development and consequences. In R. D. Ashmore & F. K. Del Boca (Eds.), The social psychology of female-male relations: A critical analysis of central concepts (pp. 21-67). New York: Academic Press.
Keith, K. D. (2011). Introduction to cross-cultural psychology. In K. D. Keith (Ed.), Cross-cultural psychology: Contemporary themes and perspectives (pp. 3-19). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Killgore W. D. S. & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2004). Activation of the amygdala and anterior cingulate during nonconscious processing of sad versus happy faces. NeuroImage, 21(4), 1215-1223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.033
Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W. & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130-149. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130
Marrs, H., Sigler, E. A. & Brammer, R. D. (2012). Gender, masculinity, femininity, and help seeking in college. Masculinities and Social Change, 1(3), 267-292. http://dx.doi.org/10.4471/mcs.2012.16
McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2012). Gender differences in reading motivation: Does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 328-336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x
Mead, M. (1935). Sex and temperament in three primitive societies. New York: Dell.
Meyenn, B. & Parker, J. (2001). Naughty boys at school: Perspectives on boys and discipline. In W. Martino & B. Meyenn (Eds.), What about the boys? Issues of masculinities in schools. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Norton, B. (2000). Identity and language learning: Gender, ethnicity and educational change. Harlow, England: Pearson Education.
Paechter, C. (2001). Using poststructuralist ideas in gender theory and research. In B. Francis & C. Skelton (Eds.), Investigating gender: Contemporary perspectives in education (pp. 41-51). Buckingham: Open University Press.
Pajares, F. & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motivation and achievement of middle school students: A function of gender orientation? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 366-381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2000.1069
Paver, B. & Gammie, E. (2005). Constructed gender, approach to learning and academic performance. Accounting Education: An International Journal, 14(4), 427-444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/06939280500347142
Persson, R. S. (1999). Exploring the meaning of gender: Evaluating and revising the Bem Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI) for a Swedish research context (BSRI-SE). INSIKT. Jönköping: HögskolanFörLärarut-BildningOchKommunikation, 1. http://swepub.kb.se/bib/swepub:oai:DiVA.org:hj-11917?tab2=abs&language=en
Pishghadam, R. (2011). Introducing applied ELT as a new paradigm. Iranian EFL Journal, 7(2), 9-20. http://profdoc.um.ac.ir/paper-abstract-1022198.html
Pishghadam, R., Baghaei, P. & Shahriari, H. (2011). Development and validation of an English language teacher competency test using item response theory. The International Journal of Educational and Psychological Assessment, 8(2), 54-68. http://profdoc.um.ac.ir/articles/a/1021894.pdf
Pishghadam, R., Golparvar, S. E. & Khajavy, G. H. (2013). The role of narrative intelligence in English language teaching, major and gender. Porta Linguarum, 19(1), 59-70. http://profdoc.um.ac.ir/paper-abstract-1031958.html
Pishghadam, R. & Khajavy, G. H. (2014). Development and validation of the Student Stroke Scale and examining its relation with academic motivation. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 43, 109-114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2014.03.004
Pishghadam, R. & Khosropanah, F. (2011). Examining construct validation of the English language competency test. International Education Studies, 4(3), 194-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ies.v4n3p194
Pishghadam R. & Mirzaee A. (2008). English language teaching in postmodern era. Teaching English Language and Literature Society of Iran, 2(7), 89-109. http://en.journals.sid.ir/ViewPaper.aspx?ID=162202
Pishghadam, R. & Moafian, F. (2008). The role of Iranian EFL teachers' multiple intelligences in their success in language teaching at high schools. Pazhuhesh-e-Zabanha-ye-Khareji, 42, 5-22. http://en.journals.sid.ir/ViewPaper.aspx?ID=104912
Pishghadam, R. & Sabouri, F. (2011). A quantitative survey on Iranian English learners' attitudes toward varieties of English: world English or world Englishes? English Language and Literature Studies, 1(1), 86-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ells.v1n1p86
Pishghadam, R. & Saboori, F. (2011). A qualitative analysis of ELT in the language institutes of Iran in the light of the theory of 'World Englishes'. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 2(3), 569-579. http://profdoc.um.ac.ir/paper-abstract-1021892.html
Pishghadam, R. & Saboori, F. (2014). A socio-cultural study of language teacher status. International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 2(1), 63-72. http://www.ijscl.net/article_4520_848.html
Pishghadam, R., Shayesteh, S. & Shapoori, M. (2011). Validation of an NLP scale and its relationship with teacher success in high schools. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 2(4), 909-917. http://profdoc.um.ac.ir/paper-abstract-1018974.html
Pishghadam, R., Zabihi, R. & Shayesteh, S. (2013). The perception-practicum interface revisited: Life-wise language teaching perceptions and teacher burnout. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 23(2), 287-297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40299-013-0104-y
Sifuna, D. N., Chege F. N. & Oanda, I. O. (2006). Themes in the study of the foundations of education. Nairobi: Jomo Kenyatta Foundation.
Spence, J. T. & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity & femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, & antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (1993). What is the relation of gender to biology and environment? An evolutionary model of how what you answer depends on just what you ask. In A. E. Beall & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (pp. 1-8). London: Guilford Press.
Stets, J. E. & Burke, P. J. (2000). Femininity/masculinity. In E. F. Borgatta & R. J. V. Montgomery (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sociology (pp. 997-1005). New York: Macmillan.
Stromquist, N. P. (2007). The gender socialization process in schools: A cross-national comparison. Paper commissioned for the EFA Global Monitoring Report 2008, Education for All by 2015: will we make it? UNESCO. http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0015/001555/155587e.pdf
Weiner, G. (2005). Bilateral vision: Gender and education in United Kingdom and Sweden. European Education, 36(4), 22-39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10564934.2004.11042370
Wheeless, V. E. & Potorti, P. F. (1989). Student assessment of teacher masculinity and femininity: A test of the sex role congruency hypothesis on student attitudes toward learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 259-262. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-0663.81.2.259
Williams, J. E. & Best, D. L. (1989). Sex and psyche: Self-concept viewed cross-culturally. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Younger, M. & Warrington, M. (2008). The gender agenda in primary teacher education in England: Fifteen lost years? Journal of Education Policy, 23(4), 429-445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02680930802054396
Feature | Teacher | |||
English | Arabic | Persian | ||
1 | Kind | |||
2 | Happy | |||
3 | Strict | |||
4 | Good-tempered | |||
5 | Honest | |||
6 | Harsh | |||
7 | Emotional | |||
8 | Active | |||
9 | Bad-tempered | |||
10 | Soft-spoken | |||
11 | Likeable | |||
12 | Self-confident | |||
13 | Sympathetic | |||
14 | Unpredictable | |||
15 | Daring | |||
16 | Friendly | |||
17 | Unsystematic | |||
18 | Athletic | |||
19 | Sensitive | |||
20 | Secretive | |||
21 | Reasonable | |||
22 | Timid | |||
23 | Reliable | |||
24 | Direct | |||
25 | Shy | |||
26 | Funny | |||
27 | Assertive | |||
28 | Patient | |||
29 | Passive | |||
30 | Masterful |
Authors: Dr Reza Pishghadam is a professor of language education at Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Iran. Email: pishghadam@um.ac.ir Dr Fahime Saboori Email: fahime.saboori@gmail.com Laila Samavarchi is a PhD candidate at Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Iran. Email: laila_samavarchi@yahoo.com Tahereh Hassanzadeh is a PhD candidate at Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Iran. Email: hassanzadeh78@gmail.com Please cite as: Pishghadam, R., Saboori, F., Samavarch. L. & Hassanzadeh, T. (2016). Examining the gender identity of language teachers using a masculinity-femininity scale: A case from Iran. Issues in Educational Research, 26(1), 131-146. http://www.iier.org.au/iier26/pishghadam.html |